Skip to main content

Maternal sensitivity and social support protect against childhood atopic dermatitis

Abstract

Background

Many studies have identified associations between qualities of maternal–child relationships and childhood asthma, but few have examined associations with childhood atopic dermatitis (AD), a common precursor to asthma. Moreover, maternal psychological distress, including prenatal and postnatal depression, anxiety and stress, may increase risk, while social support from partners may reduce risk for childhood AD. We sought to uncover the association between maternal–infant relationship qualities (maternal sensitivity towards infant behavioral signals, controlling behavior, and unresponsiveness) and child AD after accounting for risk (i.e., prenatal and postnatal maternal depression, anxiety and stress) and protective (i.e., social support) factors.

Methods

We conducted a secondary analysis of data collected on a subsample of 242 women and their infants enrolled during pregnancy in the ongoing Alberta Pregnancy Outcomes and Nutrition cohort study. Inclusion criteria required mothers to be >16 years of age, English speaking and <22 weeks gestational age at enrollment. Data on depression, anxiety and stress in the prenatal and postnatal periods and physician diagnosis of childhood AD at 18 months were gathered via maternal report. Maternal sensitivity, unresponsiveness and controlling behaviours were assessed via videotaped observations using the Child-Adult Relationship Experimental (CARE)-Index at 6 months of infant age.

Results

Higher maternal sensitivity, or the inability of the mother to appropriately understand and respond to infant needs based on behavioral signals, predicted reduced odds of AD independent of and in combination with low prenatal and postnatal anxiety and high paternal support. After adjustment, higher maternal controlling behaviours and unresponsiveness also predicted greater odds of AD.

Conclusions

Low maternal sensitivity is a risk factor for childhood AD, independently and in combination with perinatal anxiety and low social support. Thus, interventions that improve maternal–infant relationship quality, especially sensitivity, reduce anxiety and improve social support from partners could reduce odds of childhood AD.

Background

Atopic dermatitis (AD), a hypersensitive skin disease characterized by inflamed and scaly skin lesions, pruritus and skin rash is one of the most common chronic diseases to affect children in Western society [1]. Symptoms often appear by 6 months of age, but 65% of affected children will have symptoms by 18 months of age and in more than 50% of children, symptoms will persist through 7 years of age [2]. AD symptoms often lead to depression, sleep deprivation, feelings of embarrassment, stigma, social isolation, and restricted ability to own pets or play sports [2, 3]. Consequently, AD leads to a diminished quality of life for children [3] and its medical care burdens children, parents and the health care system [4, 5]. Substantial attention has been paid to understanding the risk factors for AD and their implications in the atopic march toward allergic rhinitis and ultimately asthma [2, 6,7,8,9,10,11]. Given that AD is a potent early warning sign for later atopic diseases, ascertaining qualities of the early environment that may predispose children to AD is warranted.

A recent systematic review identified early environmental risk factors for childhood AD to be maternal prenatal psychological distress, including depression, anxiety and stress [12]. A study of 1264 mother–infant pairs revealed that maternal report of prenatal mental status including vitality, vigor, happiness, anxiety, discouragement, nervousness, tiredness, exhaustion and work stress predicted AD in 2 year olds [13]. A large (n = 23,791) cross-sectional study revealed that parental history of diagnosed prenatal and postnatal depression predicted AD in 6–13 year olds [14]. Maternal depressive symptoms in the postnatal period were associated with AD in 3–12 month old infants as was postnatal anxiety, particularly about childrearing (n = 40) [15]. In several other studies, maternal prenatal stressful life events such as divorce, mourning the death of a loved one, job loss or financial problems were associated with AD in 3–14 year olds, independent of sociodemographic factors or known atopy risk factors, like allergen exposure [16,17,18]. Stressful life events during the second half of gestation were found to increase the odds of AD in 14 year olds [17]. To our knowledge studies have not examined the differential impact of prenatal or postnatal depression, anxiety and stress on the development of childhood AD.

Although prenatal and postnatal depression, anxiety and stress are often co-morbid [19,20,21], they likely exert unique contributions to AD development. Postnatally depressed mothers often fail to respond appropriately to infant cues, alleviate infant stress and overall, are less sensitive and responsive towards their infants [22,23,24]. This propensity is widely held responsible for the poor developmental outcomes often observed in children of depressed mothers [25, 26]. Prenatal psychological distress may also negatively affect the priming of the maternal brain for sensitive, responsive interactions with infants [27]. MaternalFootnote 1 sensitivity is defined as a pattern of behaviour that pleases the infant and increases the infant’s comfort and attentiveness and reduces its distress and disengagement [28]. Consistently alleviating infant distress by responding to behavioral signals, such as fussiness due to hunger or fatigue, in a timely fashion is an indicator of high maternal sensitivity, while failing to do so is indicative of low responsiveness [29]. Moreover, high maternal sensitivity and responsiveness are contrary to maternal behaviours that are overtly or covertly hostile or attempt to overly control the infants’ behaviours in everyday interactions [30]. As a result, healthy maternal–infant relationships are typically characterized as sensitive and responsive interactions that are attentive to the infants’ needs while remaining non-intrusive and non-controlling, and contribute to the regulation of the infant’s response to stress before the infant is physiologically able to self-regulate [31,32,33]. In other words, infants develop their ability to regulate their stress response through their relationship with their maternal caregivers, especially during the first 3 years of life [34, 35]. Caregivers who do not provide sensitive, responsive, non-controlling care promote development of precocious self-regulation, which may have physiological costs, including suppressed immune development [36, 37].

As early as 1999, maternal–infant relationships were theorized to predict and/or compound childhood AD [38]; however, to date, only two studies have examined this theorized association [15, 39]. In contrast, many studies have identified associations between maternal–infant relationships and childhood asthma [40,41,42,43,44,45]. One study compared children with (n = 20) and without (n = 20) AD, they found that self-reported maternal overprotectiveness and control were associated with AD in 3–12 month olds [15]. Another study compared self-reported relationship characteristics of mothers of children less than 6 years of age with (n = 102) and without (n = 131) AD and found that mothers of children with AD were less affectionate and that mothers of children with more severe symptoms were more rejecting and less encouraging of children’s autonomy [39]. They also examined maternal sensitivity via parental self-report and failed to find any difference between AD and non-AD groups [39]. However, self-report measures of maternal–infant relationship quality are poorly correlated with observational measures [46] and thus observations of are much preferred [47, 48], yet much ignored in the research on AD or other atopic diseases. Given the role of AD in the atopic march and the consistent observation of associations between maternal–infant relationships and childhood asthma, and emerging evidence of associations with AD, ascertaining the influence of qualities of the maternal–child relationship (sensitivity, responsiveness, and control) using observational measures, on AD would be useful.

Furthermore, existing research has not yet examined protective factors in AD development. Social support has been described as a buffer to stressful life events, enhancing the mother’s self-esteem and self-efficacy and aiding in the transition to motherhood, consequently promoting healthy child development [49, 50]. Social support refers to emotional, affirmational, informational and instrumental assistance, from social relationships including partners [51]. Without appropriate social support during the transition to motherhood, this transition can be difficult and distressing, adding to maternal psychological distress and affecting the mother’s ability to care for her infant [49]. Lack of social support has been strongly associated with maternal depressive symptoms [51, 52]. Research has affirmed the protective role of social support, particularly from fathers, for normative and depressed mothers [53,54,55,56]. To our knowledge, existing research has not yet examined maternal social support as a protective factor in children’s AD. Other identified risk factors include parental history of atopic disease [57, 58], especially asthma, low maternal education, being a boy [59], high birth weight and day care attendance in the first year of life [60]. Finally, breastfeeding has been shown to predict both increased maternal sensitivity [53, 61] and changes in risk of AD [62].

To address the aforementioned gaps in the literature, we sought to determine the association between maternal–infant relationship qualities (sensitivity, responsiveness and control) and child AD by 2 years of age, considering risk (i.e. maternal prenatal and postnatal depression, anxiety and stress) and protective (i.e. social support) factors. Given the prominent role of maternal sensitivity, we also sought to identity predictors of maternal sensitivity. We hypothesized that lower maternal sensitivity, higher unresponsiveness and control would increase the risk for AD in young children. We also hypothesized that greater symptoms of depression and anxiety and reported stress would predict lower maternal sensitivity and increased risk of childhood AD. Finally, we hypothesized that greater reported postnatal social support would reduce the likelihood of AD.

Methods

This is a secondary analysis of data from the Fetal Programming Study [63], a sub-study of the larger Alberta Pregnancy Outcomes and Nutrition (APrON) longitudinal pregnancy cohort study. The Fetal Programming Study was designed to examine biomarkers of maternal stress during pregnancy and to collect data on postnatal maternal–infant relationships and other health outcomes [64]. Ethics approvals were obtained from the Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board at the University of Calgary and the Health Research Ethics Board at the University of Alberta, both in Canada. After obtaining informed consent, data were collected in early (13–22 weeks) and late (32–40 weeks) gestation and again at 3 (range 2–4 months), 6 (5–8 months) and 18 (range 12–27 months) months postpartum via clinic visits and mailed questionnaires. Trained research assistants conducted clinic visits at the regional children’s hospital. No incentives to participation were offered. Details on the full cohort are published [64].

Sample and recruitment

Between 2011 and 2012, 294 participants were recruited and enrolled in the sub-study, a portion of the final total APrON cohort of 2189 mothers and their infants. See Fig. 1 for details. Due to the nature of observational measurement in the sub-sample, recruitment was constrained to Calgary. Mothers were identified via maternity, ultra sound, family medicine and obstetrics clinics and media advertisements [64]. Participant enrollment eligibility criteria included mothers <22 weeks gestational age (GA) at first study visit, >18 years of age, not smoking or drinking alcohol during pregnancy, singleton pregnancy, not on any synthetic glucocorticoid medication, and no known fetal complications during study entry [63]. At the 18 month follow-up clinic visit, the sample retained was comprised of 242 mother–infant pairs.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Enrollment of individuals through each stage of the study

Overview of study variables

This study collected data on infant AD at the 18 month clinic visit. Extensive data were collected on mothers’ demographic characteristics, health history, including maternal history of asthma, prenatal and postnatal maternal psychological distress (anxiety, depression or stress), maternal social support (specifically the extent of partner/spouse support) and children’s birth and health outcomes including breastfeeding status (at 3 months) from the APrON surveys completed at prenatal or postnatal clinic visits or sent in by mail. Observations of maternal–infant relationships were conducted at 6 month clinic visits. Covariates included identified risk factors for AD: maternal asthma, low maternal education, being a boy [59], breastfeeding status and birth weight [60]). (Another known covariate, day care attendance in the first year of life, was not included as in our Canadian sample, nearly all the children’s parents were on a 1-year parental leave.)

Infant atopic dermatitis

This was assessed via questionnaire at the 18 month clinic visit during which mothers reported physician diagnosis of infant AD. Mothers were asked to report if their child was diagnosed by a physician with AD in the last 6 months. AD was thus dichotomized as 1 (AD diagnosis) or 0 (no AD diagnosis) for analysis. In other studies, parent report has yielded high sensitivity, specificity and concordance [65] with corroborated physician diagnosis of AD.

Maternal–infant relationship quality

Maternal–infant relationship qualities at 6 months of age were assessed with the Child-Adult Relationship Experimental (CARE)-Index, a 5 min observational procedure involving videotaping the mother and child playing with age-appropriate, child-safe toys [28]. Video recordings were coded to attain scores for the constructs of maternal sensitivity, unresponsiveness and control. Possible scores range from 0 to 14, with higher scores indicating more observations of the construct. Moreover, sensitivity maps onto dyadic synchrony, a global measure of maternal–infant relationship quality, and scores may be categorized in the “sensitive” (11–14), “adequate” (7–10), “intervention” (5–6) and “at-risk” (0–4) ranges. The CARE-Index has a high degree of stability and has been well validated [66,67,68,69,70,71]. Typical inter-rater reliabilities range from r = 0.73–0.95 [70, 72]. Author Letourneau is reliable CARE-Index coder and supervised the administration of the measure. Video-recorded observations were coded by blinded, reliable coders who achieved 94.4% inter-rater reliability agreement on the dominant pattern (sensitivity, control and unresponsiveness). Disagreements were resolved by additional viewings and discussion.

Prenatal and postnatal maternal psychological distress

Mothers self-reported ratings of psychological distress (depression, anxiety and stress) via the Edinburgh Depression Scale [73, 74] (EDS), Symptom Checklist-90 item-Revised (SCL-90-R) anxiety subscale [75], Pregnancy-Specific Anxiety Scale (PSAS) [76] and Stressful Life Events Questionnaire (SLEQ) [77]. All were administered in early and late pregnancy, and again at 3 months postpartum, with the exception of the administration of the PSAS only in pregnancy. The 10-item EDS has well-demonstrated reliability and validity with test–retest reliability, and good to moderate correlation with other measures of depression [73, 78, 79]. Measuring general anxiety, the SCL-90-R includes 25 items designed to reflect a symptom inventory of general anxiety [75], for which convergent and divergent validity has been established [80]. The possible range of scores for EDS and SCL-90-R is 0–30, with higher scores indicative of more severe depression or anxiety [78, 81]. For the SCL-90-R, T-scores were tabulated, with scores greater than or equal to 63 indicating high risk for anxiety. The PSAS, a 10-item self-report instrument, is a reliable measure (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81) [54] of pregnant women’s worry regarding personal health, labor and delivery and baby care, with final scores ranging from 0 to 3. To measure the number of stressors, mothers provided self-reported ratings on the 26-item SLEQ [82, 83], indicating “yes” or “no” to whether they have experienced stressful life events, for example, a death or illness of family member or friend, divorce or separation. The range of possible scores is 0–26, with higher scores indicating a greater number of stressors. As a checklist, internal reliability or inter-item correlations are difficult to establish; however, content, construct and face validity are well recognized [82, 83].

Maternal social support

Mothers’ perceptions of the quality of their partners’ (88% of participants reported on their child’s father) social support at 3 months postnatal was assessed via the Social Support Effectiveness Questionnaire (SSEQ), a 35-item measure of emotional/affirmational, informational, instrumental and negative support received over the previous 3 months. Total scores range from 0 to 80, with higher scores indicating more effective support from partners. The internal consistency for this instrument is strong (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.87) when used to distinguish levels of social support for childbearing women [53, 84, 85].

Data analysis

The sample characteristics were described using descriptive summaries, including means and standard deviations. Predictor variables were dichotomized using percentiles or appropriate cut-offs (e.g. high/low or probable/not probable) to aid interpretability. To gain more insight into the relationship between maternal sensitivity and AD, maternal sensitivity was classified into the four standard categories: sensitive, adequate, intervention and at-risk, and scores ≥7 are considered “high” on sensitivity (adequate or sensitive) [28] and plotted against the percentage of children with AD. As no established cut-offs exist for maternal unresponsiveness and control, we used 75th percentile comparisons. Maximum values were used from prenatal and postnatal assessments of depression, anxiety and stress, to attain for example, the maximum depression score for the prenatal period and another maximum score for the postnatal period. These maximums were utilized in all statistical tests. Mean differences in maternal–infant relationship measures across the predictors were examined by using t tests and the association between AD and potential predictors was investigated using Fisher’s Chi square test. To rule out collinearity among the predictors, pairwise correlation amongst the predictors was tested using Pearson tests revealing that all the predictors were fairly or moderately (−0.35 to 0.34) correlated and suitable for inclusion in the analysis. Simple logistic regression was employed to identify potential predictors of AD. The predictors that were moderately associated (crude associations) with AD (p < 0.25) in step one and, as recommended [86], those that have previously shown to be associated with AD in other studies were selected and included in the multivariable logistic regression model. In the final analysis, two multivariable logistic models were fitted based on the study hypotheses. The first model included maternal sensitivity, maternal asthma and distress to predict AD. In the second model, maternal sensitivity was excluded and replaced with maternal unresponsiveness and control to predict AD. The goodness of fit for both models was assessed using the Pearson Chi square. Missing data were treated with listwise deletion.

Results

In this affluent study population (Table 1), prenatal depression was found in 23.6%, prenatal anxiety in 24.1%, postnatal depression in 7.1% and postnatal anxiety in 5.6% of women. Newborns were full-term, normal weight and predominantly breastfed at 3 months. Asthma was reported in 8.3% of mothers and 18.1% of their children had AD, according to maternal report of physician diagnosis at 18 months of age.

Table 1 Demographic and descriptive characteristics (n = 242)

Association between maternal–infant relationship quality at 6 months and child AD at 18 months

Mean scores on the CARE-Index were 5.0 (SD = 1.9) for maternal sensitivity, 6.3 (SD = 3.7) for unresponsiveness and 2.7 (SD = 3.5) for controlling in the context of maternal–infant relationships. Correlations revealed that maternal sensitivity was negatively correlated with the controlling (r = −0.19) and unresponsive (r = −0.33) domains of the CARE-Index.

Table 2 outlines descriptive and unadjusted associations among predictors, covariates and both maternal sensitivity at 6 months and AD at 18 months. Significant predictors of high maternal sensitivity (21.3% of sample) were age of women, with women over 30 years being more sensitive, and higher postnatal depressive symptoms, with the mean sensitivity score (M = 9.50, SD = 1.91) of highly symptomatic mothers being higher than the mean sensitivity score (M = 7.63, SD = 1.07) of less symptomatic mothers. The only significant predictor of AD at age 18 months (18% of sample) was maternal history of asthma.

Table 2 Percentage distribution of descriptive and predictor variables for mothers with high maternal sensitivity at 6 months and child AD at 18 months (Chi square tests)

Figure 2 shows that AD was reported in almost 25% of toddlers born to mothers scoring in the ‘at risk’ range of the maternal CARE-Index sensitivity measure, then dropped successively with increases to maternal sensitivity to a prevalence of less than 1% among toddlers of sensitive mothers.

Fig. 2
figure 2

The more sensitive the mothers in the maternal–infant relationship observations, the fewer children were identified with AD. Total N = 242

Association between maternal sensitivity, maternal distress, social support and maternal asthma and child AD at 18 months

Table 3 presents these data translated into odd ratios; maternal sensitivity was negatively associated with AD, such that this allergic skin disorder decreased in offspring by 0.81 for each unit increase in the maternal sensitivity score. Mothers with a history of asthma were also 3.4 times more likely to have a child with AD. When adjusted for maternal asthma status, prenatal or postnatal anxiety, and postnatal depression, the statistical association between maternal sensitivity and childhood AD remained; the odds ratio for AD further decreased to 0.74 for each unit increase in the maternal sensitivity score. In this model, maternal asthma and postnatal anxiety independently increased the risk for AD. The likelihood of AD was 0.73 for each unit increase in maternal sensitivity in a final model that also included postnatal social support. In this fully adjusted model, child AD was significantly more likely with prenatal or postnatal anxiety and maternal asthma status; it was inversely related to postnatal depression and postnatal social support. Neither prenatal nor postnatal stress contributed to the models.

Table 3 Maternal sensitivity, maternal distress, social support and maternal asthma associations with AD at 18 months

Maternal responsiveness and control, maternal distress, maternal asthma and social support associations with AD at 18 months

Between 20 and 25% of children had AD when mothers exhibited unresponsive or controlling variants of the maternal–infant relationship, whereas they were 15% in their absence (Fig. 3). Unadjusted odds ratios for associations between unresponsiveness and controlling behaviour and AD were not significant; however, in a fully-adjusted model, independent of maternal asthma, postnatal depression or social support, child AD was 1.3–1.4 times more likely in the presence of maternal unresponsive and controlling behaviour (Table 4). As with the maternal sensitivity model, AD was independently associated with maternal asthma status, with prenatal and postnatal maternal anxiety, and inversely associated with postnatal depression and social support. Neither prenatal nor postnatal stress contributed to the models.

Fig. 3
figure 3

At high levels of maternal control/unresponsiveness in mother–infant relationship observations, more children have AD than children whose mothers are less controlling/more responsive. Total N = 242

Table 4 Maternal responsiveness and control, maternal distress, maternal asthma and social support associations with AD at 18 months

Discussion

In a subsample of 242 women and their infants enrolled in the APrON longitudinal study, high maternal sensitivity was found to significantly protect against AD development at 18 months of age (adjusted OR = 0.73; 95% CI 0.56, 0.93, p = 0.012), independent of maternal asthma status. The maternal sensitivity and child AD association was also independent of maternal prenatal and postnatal anxiety, and social support. A mother’s assessment of the quality of support provided by her partner reduced the onset of this early childhood atopic disorder by 0.96 for every unit increase of perceived support. Maternal anxiety was a risk factor for AD in offspring. Further, positive associations with child AD were observed for maternal controlling and unresponsive behaviors (adjusted OR = 1.33, 95% CI 1.03, 1.71, p = 0.028 and adjusted OR = 1.35, 95% CI 1.05, 1.73, p = 0.020 respectively). Maternal sensitivity was predicted by maternal age above 30 and, in contrast to expectations, higher postnatal depressive symptoms.

Our results suggest that poor maternal–infant relationship quality (low sensitive, high unresponsive and high controlling behavior during interactions with infants) at 6 months of age increases the risk of child AD at 18 months of age. Our findings are consistent with evidence demonstrating the link between maternal–infant relationship quality and child atopic disease, although current understanding is largely limited to the etiology of asthma [40,41,42,43, 45]. For example, identified difficulties in the parent–child relationship independently predict asthma onset at 3 and 6 years of age [41], while the onset of asthma in early life has been associated with poor quality relationships with parents that persist into adulthood [87].

Infants whose caregivers are less sensitive have higher levels of cortisol [33, 88] and excess cortisol exposure during early infancy may influence the developing immune response via epigenetics or neuroendocrine dysregulation, potentially leading to AD [87, 89, 90]. It is also possible that more sensitive mothers take greater care of their infants’ skin, by for example, use of moisturizer and regular bathing; however, how this may contribute to the atopic march toward asthma is unclear. To our knowledge, no evidence has been published demonstrating associations among maternal sensitivity, infant skin care, and the atopic march, but these associations are worth exploration as possible rival hypotheses. A recent review summarizing the increasing literature on links between maternal–infant relationships and asthma development, noted that only correlational, not causal links have been established. The review called for randomized controlled trials of social interventions focused on maternal–infant relationships with examination of atopic disease outcomes [44].

Predictors of maternal sensitivity typically include low postpartum depression, anxiety, and stress, high paternal support and adult (as opposed to adolescent) motherhood [91,92,93]. Depression is known to deplete maternal ability to express positive affect towards her infant, and could possibly be exacerbated by lack of paternal support [91]. Our finding that older adult mothers are more sensitive to their infants is in keeping with research suggesting that younger mothers engage less frequently than older adult mothers in behaviors that promote secure attachment, such as positive verbal feedback [94]. But, contrary to much evidence [61, 62, 90, 92, 93, 95], neither maternal stress, anxiety nor breastfeeding were significantly associated with maternal sensitivity. We also observed an unexpected association between higher postnatal depressive symptoms and maternal sensitivity, which may have been due to small sample size. Women reporting excessive depression, anxiety or stress are typically less positively reinforcing and interactive with their infant, thought to lead to poor child development outcomes [22,23,24, 96]. However, new data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (n = 704) revealed that perinatal depressive symptoms predicted little variance in maternal–infant relationship qualities, but rather maternal interpersonal sensitivity in pregnancy is the most significant contributor to forming healthy maternal–infant relationships in the context of perinatal depressive symptoms [97]. In other words, other variables that are correlated with both depressive symptoms and qualities of the maternal–child relationship may underpin observed associations between depression and child development and deserve exploration. Moreover, depressive and anxiety symptoms in pregnancy are typically worse than in the postpartum period [98, 99], so significant differences would be expected; however, how these differences may influence maternal–child relationships or onset of children’s AD is unknown. Studies have shown that high levels of perceived support reported by depressed or anxious mothers may buffer the association with child atopic disease, suggesting accessible support may improve the quality of the mother–child relationship [100,101,102]. In this study, although greater reported postnatal social support was shown to reduce the likelihood of AD, consistent with various studies [34, 53, 54], social support was not found to be a predictor of sensitive maternal behavior.

Prenatal pregnancy-specific anxiety (adjusted OR = 2.74; 95% CI 1.04, 7.19, p = 0.041) and postnatal anxiety (adjusted OR = 1.16, 95% CI 1.01, 1.33, p = 0.034) predicted AD independent of paternal support and maternal sensitivity. A trend for an inverse association between postnatal depression and AD was also observed. These findings are compatible with several studies on childhood atopic disease associations with maternal prenatal or postnatal stressors [13, 16,17,18, 59, 103], prenatal anxiety but not depression, as suggested above [88, 104,105,106,107]. Mutual adjustment for anxiety and depression may have uncovered the negative influence of postnatal depression on the health care utilization for infant care and thus, physician diagnosis of AD [108]. Consistent with our results, some studies found postnatal anxiety about childrearing to be associated with AD in 3–12 month old infants [15]. As maternal anxiety becomes more prevalent [109], it may gain a more prominent influence on the maternal–infant relationship [97]. Given that low maternal sensitivity and anxiety are interrelated, the association with childhood AD may be similarly mediated by epigenetics or neuroendocrine dysregulation [88, 104, 110], and altered infant cytokine profiles [111]. Infant neuroendocrine and immunologic maturation is highly plastic in the face of environmental stressors [112,113,114,115], especially during the early postnatal period. For those who are already genetically susceptible to the development of atopy [116], as observed in our sample of children whose mothers reported a history of asthma, maternal sensitivity may make the difference between the development of AD or not.

The pathogenesis of AD is known to involve abnormal levels of specific cytokines released by T-helper 2 cells detected in cord blood or infant peripheral blood [117,118,119,120]. The altered differentiation of these cytokines may be induced through excessive glucocorticoid exposure at an early age, driven by maternal perinatal psychosocial distress [121, 122]. Future research could focus on understanding the associations among: (1) excessive, long-term fetal/infant glucocorticoid exposure during critical developmental periods, (2) Th2 specific cytokine levels in infant cord or peripheral blood, and (3) fetal programming, driven by prenatal distress (anxiety, depression and stress), and (4) poor maternal–infant relationship qualities, linked to postnatal distress. Moreover, genetic risk, suggested by the heritability of atopic disease suggest a need for genome-wide association studies [13].

This study has many strengths, including the outcome of childhood AD assessed via maternal report of a physician diagnosis of AD and observational assessment of maternal–infant relationship quality, but there are also limitations. First, physician corroboration of maternal report of physician diagnosis of AD was not possible. Second, only “maternal” caregiving was assessed; rather than seeking to reinforce gender stereotypes, we recognize that primary caregivers may be mothers, fathers or others. However, we also recognize that the vast majority of infants’ primary caregivers are mothers in Canada [123] and in our study [64]. Third, only a small number of mothers reported depressive symptoms in the postpartum period. This may have contributed to the potentially spurious findings of inverse association between postpartum depression and outcomes. Additionally, not all participants reported perceptions of the quality of their partners’ support, as only 88% of participants reported on their child’s father. Nonetheless, the variability in primary support provider may also reflect the reality of modern families. Furthermore, while we collected data on maternal asthma, we did not collect additional data on mother’s atopy status or family history of atopy. Finally, we acknowledge that this is a relatively high SES sample with low sociodemographic risk that may limit generalizability.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to uncover the association between maternal–infant relationship qualities (sensitivity, control, unresponsiveness) and childhood AD after accounting for risk (maternal depression, anxiety and stress) and protective (social support) factors and well-known covariates. We found low maternal sensitivity to be a significant risk factor for childhood AD, in the presence and absence of perinatal anxiety and low social support. Caregivers who are emotionally engaged and supportive of their children during infancy may be well equipped to prevent poor infant immunological development, reducing the likelihood of childhood AD and potentially asthma [34]. Limited work has begun to establish that interventions focused on the quality of maternal child-relationship may influence asthma symptoms [45]. These results suggest interventions that improve maternal–infant relationship quality, anxiety and support could reduce odds of childhood AD. Existing parenting intervention programs, including the Nurse Family Partnership [124] and Keys to Caregiving [125], and support services may already have effects on AD and the atopic march, that have not to date been measured. In addition, strengthening of relationships between caregivers serves as a means of controlling maternal anxiety [95] and may therefore be effective in helping prevent childhood AD. Additional intervention studies ought to focus on improving maternal child relationship quality and reducing odds of AD, as a means to forestall the atopic march to allergic rhinitis and ultimately asthma.

Conclusions

High maternal sensitivity and social support, in combination with low perinatal anxiety, are associated with reduced odds of childhood AD. Higher maternal control and unresponsiveness are associated with increased odds of childhood AD. Clinical assessment of perinatal anxiety, improved social support and the introduction of early intervention programs designed to improve maternal–infant relationship quality may reduce the odds of childhood atopic dermatitis and the progression towards the atopic march, as well as help uncover genetic factors and explanatory neuroendocrine and epigenetic mechanisms.

Notes

  1. We recognize that mothers are not the only caregivers for children and that relationships with primary caregivers and children are paramount, regardless of gender; however, all research to date and reviewed here has focused on mothers.

Abbreviations

AD:

atopic dermatitis

APrON:

Alberta Pregnancy Outcomes and Nutrition

GA:

gestational age

CARE-Index:

Child-Adult Relationship Experimental-Index

EDS:

Edinburgh Depression Scale

SCL-90-R:

Symptom Checklist-90 item-Revised

PSAS:

Pregnancy-Specific Anxiety Scale

SLEQ:

Stressful Life Events Questionnaire

SSEQ:

Social Support Effectiveness Questionnaire

References

  1. Draaisma E, et al. A multinational study to compare prevalence of atopic dermatitis in the first year of life. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2015;26(4):359–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Spergel JM. Epidemiology of atopic dermatitis and atopic march in children. Immunol Allergy Clin N Am. 2010;30(3):269–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Lewis-Jones S. Quality of life and childhood atopic dermatitis: the misery of living with childhood eczema. Int J Clin Pract. 2006;60(8):984–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Carroll CL, et al. The burden of atopic dermatitis: impact on the patient, family, and society. Pediatr Dermatol. 2005;22(3):192–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Stewart M, et al. Support needs and preferences of young adolescents with asthma and allergies: “just no one really seems to understand”. J Pediatr Nurs. 2012;27(5):479–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Spergel JM, Paller AS. Atopic dermatitis and the atopic march. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2003;112(6):S118–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Spergel JM. From atopic dermatitis to asthma: the atopic march. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol. 2010;105(2):99–106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Cantani A. The growing genetic links and the early onset of atopic diseases in children stress the unique role of the atopic march: a meta-analysis. J Investig Allergol Clin Immunol. 1998;9(5):314–20.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Hahn EL, Bacharier LB. The atopic march: the pattern of allergic disease development in childhood. Immunol Allergy Clin N Am. 2005;25(2):231–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Lowe AJ, et al. Do boys do the atopic march while girls dawdle? J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2008;121(5):1190–5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Ker J, Hartert TV. The atopic march: what’s the evidence? Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol. 2009;103(4):282–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Andersson N, et al. Prenatal maternal stress and atopic diseases in the child: a systematic review of observational human studies. Allergy. 2016;71(1):15–26.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Wen HJ, et al. Prediction of atopic dermatitis in 2-yr-old children by cord blood IgE, genetic polymorphisms in cytokine genes, and maternal mentality during pregnancy. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2011;22(7):695–703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Li F, et al. Prevalence and risk factors of childhood allergic diseases in eight metropolitan cities in China: a multicenter study. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):437.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Pauli-Pott U, Darui A, Beckmann D. Infants with atopic dermatitis: maternal hopelessness, child rearing attitudes and perceived infant temperament. Psychother Psychosom. 1999;68(1):39–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. de Marco R, et al. Foetal exposure to maternal stressful events increases the risk of having asthma and atopic diseases in childhood. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2012;23(8):724–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Hartwig IR, et al. Prenatal adverse life events increase the risk for atopic diseases in children, which is enhanced in the absence of a maternal atopic predisposition. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2014;134(1):160–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bockelbrink A, Heinrich J, Schafer I, Zutavern A, Borte M, Herbath O, Schaaf B, von Berg A, Schafer T. Atopic eczema in children: another harmful sequel of divorce. Allergy. 2006;61(12):1397–402.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Grigoriadis S, de Camps Meschino D, Barrons E. Mood and anxiety disorders in a sample of perinatal women referred for psychiatric care. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2011;14:325–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Gaynes B, et al. Perinatal depression: prevalence, screening, accuracy, and screening outcomes. Research Triangle Park: Agency for Healthcare Research Quality; 2005.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Batenburg-Eddes V, et al. Parental depressive and anxiety symptoms during pregnancy and attention problems in children: a cross-cohort consistency study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54(5):591–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(3–4):329–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Pearson R, et al. Depressive symptoms in early pregnancy disrupt attentional processing of infant emotion. Psychol Med. 2010;40(04):621–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Warnock FF, et al. Caregiving behavior and interactions of prenatally depressed mothers (antidepressant-treated and non-antidepressant-treated) during newborn acute pain. Infant Ment Health J. 2009;30(4):384–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Letourneau N, et al. Postpartum depression is a family affair: addressing the impact on mothers, fathers, and children. Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2012;33(7):445–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Field T. Postpartum depression effects on early interactions, parenting and safety practices: a review. Infant Behav Dev. 2011;33:1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Glynn LM, Sandman CA. Prenatal origins of neurological development a critical period for fetus and mother. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2011;20(6):384–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Crittenden P. CARE-index infants coding manual. Miami: Family Relations Institute; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Barnard K, Guralnick MJ. Influencing parent–child interactions for children at risk. In: The effectiveness of early interventions. Toronto: Paul Brooks; 1997. p. 99–126.

  30. Kelly JF, Zuckerman T, Rosenblatt S. Promoting first relationships: a relationship-focused early intervention approach. Infants Young Child. 2008;21(4):285–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Conradt E, Ablow J. Infant physiological response to the still-face paradigm: contributions of maternal sensitivity and infants’ early regulatory behavior. Infant Behav Dev. 2010;33(3):251–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Sroufe LA. Attachment and development: a prospective, longitudinal study from birth to adulthood. Attach Hum Dev. 2005;7(4):349–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Letourneau N, et al. Cortisol patterns of depressed mothers and their infants are related to maternal–infant interactive behaviours. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2011;29(5):439–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Suglia SF, et al. Maternal intimate partner violence and increased asthma incidence in children: buffering effects of supportive caregiving. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(3):244–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Thomas JC, et al. Developmental origins of infant emotion regulation: mediation by temperamental negativity and moderation by maternal sensitivity. Dev Psychol. 2017;53(4):611.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Hellyer J, et al. Are maternal and community risk factors associated with the presence of asthma among children placed in foster care? Child Youth Serv Rev. 2013;35(1):128–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Thompson RA. Stress and child development. Future Child. 2014;24(1):41–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Howlett S. Emotional dysfunction, child–family relationships and childhood atopic dermatitis. Br J Dermatol. 1999;140:381–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Im Y-J, et al. Parenting and relationship characteristics in mothers with their children having atopic disease. J Child Health Care. 2014;18(3):215–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Cassibba R, et al. Attachment of mothers and children with recurrent asthmatic bronchitis. J Asthma. 2004;41(4):419–31.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Klinnert M, et al. Onset and persistence of childhood asthma: predictors from infancy. Pediatrics. 2001;108(4):69–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Mrazek D. Insecure attachment in severely asthmatic preschool children: is it a risk factor? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1987;26(4):516–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Mrazek D, et al. Prediction of early-onset asthma in genetically at-risk children. Pediatr Pulmonol. 1999;27(2):85–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Yatsenko O, Pizano J, Nikolaidis A. Revisiting maternal–infant bonding’s effects on asthma: a brief history. Cogent Psychol. 2016;3(1):1161267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Madrid A. Helping children with asthma by repairing maternal–infant bonding problems. Am J Clin Hypn. 2006;48(2–3):199–2011.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Morsbach SK, Prinz RJ. Understanding and improving the validity of self-report of parenting. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2006;9(1):1–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Gardner F. Methodological issues in the direct observation of parent–child interaction: do observational findings reflect the natural behavior of participants? Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2000;3(3):185–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Aspland H, Gardner F. Observational measures of parent–child interaction: an introductory review. Child Adolesc Ment Health. 2003;8(3):136–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Emmanuel EN, et al. Maternal role development: the impact of maternal distress and social support following childbirth. Midwifery. 2011;27(2):265–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Fahey JO, Shenassa E. Understanding and meeting the needs of women in the postpartum period: the perinatal maternal health promotion model. J Midwifery Womens Health. 2013;58(6):613–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Eastwood JG, et al. Relationship of postnatal depressive symptoms to infant temperament, maternal expectations, social support and other potential risk factors: findings from a large Australian cross-sectional study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2012;12(1):1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Leung BM, et al. Perinatal nutrition in maternal mental health and child development: birth of a pregnancy cohort. Early Hum Dev. 2016;93:1–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Rini C, Schetter CD, Glynn L, Hobel C, Sandman C. Effective social support: antecedents and consequences of partner support during pregnancy. Pers Relatsh. 2006;13:207–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Rini C, Schetter CD. The effectiveness of social support in intimate relationships. In: Davila KSJ, editor. Support processes in intimate relationships. New York: Oxford; 2010. p. 26–67.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  55. Letourneau N, et al. Postpartum depression: maternal and service providers’ perspectives of support needs. In: International institute for qualitative methodology conference, Edmonton, AB. 2005.

  56. Dennis C, Letourneau N. Global and relationship-specific perceptions of support and the development of postpartum depressive symptomatology. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2007;42(5):389–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Purvis D, et al. Risk factors for atopic dermatitis in New Zealand children at 3–5 years of age. Br J Dermatol. 2005;152(4):742–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Doğruel D, Bingöl G, Altıntaş DU, Yılmaz M, Kendirli SG. Prevalence of and risk factors for atopic dermatitis: a birth cohort study of infants in southeast Turkey. Allergol Immunopathol. 2016;44(3):214–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Wen HJ, et al. Predicting risk for early infantile atopic dermatitis by hereditary and environmental factors. Br J Dermatol. 2009;161(5):1166–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Kerkhof M, et al. Risk factors for atopic dermatitis in infants at high risk of allergy: the PIAMA study. Clin Exp Allergy. 2003;33(10):1336–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Tharner A, et al. Breastfeeding and its relation to maternal sensitivity and infant attachment. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2012;33(5):396–404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Benn CS, et al. Breastfeeding and risk of atopic dermatitis, by parental history of allergy, during the first 18 months of life. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;160(3):217–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Giesbrecht GF, Letourneau N, Campbell TS. Sexually dimorphic and interactive effects of prenatal maternal cortisol and psychological distress on infant cortisol reactivity. Dev Psychopathol. 2016. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000493.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Kaplan BJ, et al. The Alberta pregnancy outcomes and nutrition (APrON) cohort study: rationale and methods. Matern Child Nutr. 2014;10(1):44–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Von Kobyletzki LB, Janson S, Hasselgren M, Bornehag CG, Svensson Å. Evaluation of a parental questionnaire to identify atopic dermatitis in infants and preschool children. J Allergy. 2012;2012:945617.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Crittenden P, Bonvillian J. The effect of maternal risk status on maternal sensitivity to infant cues. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1984;54:250–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. DiLalla D, Crittenden P. Dimensions of maltreated children’s home behavior: a factor analytic approach. Infant Behav Dev. 1990;13:439–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Ward M, Carlson E. Associations among adult attachment representations, maternal sensitivity, and infant–mother attachment in a sample of adolescent mothers. Child Dev. 1995;66:69–79.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Leadbeater BJ, Bishop S, Raver C. Quality of mother–toddler interaction, maternal depressive symptoms, and behavioral problems of adolescent mothers. Dev Psychol. 1996;32:280–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Leventhal A, et al. Caregiver attitudes and at-risk maternal behavior among mothers with major mental illness. Psychiatr Serv. 2004;55:1431–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Crittenden P, DiLalla D. Compulsive compliance: the development of an inhibitory coping strategy in infancy. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1988;16:585–99.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Azar R, et al. The association of major depression, conduct disorder, and maternal overcontrol with a failure to show a cortisol buffered response in 4-month-old infants of teenage mothers. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(6):573–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R. Detection of postnatal depression: development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150:782–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Matthey S. Differentiating between transient and enduring distress on the Edinburgh Depression Scale within screening contexts. J Affect Disord. 2016;196:252–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Derogatis L. Symptom checklist-90-R: administration, scoring and procedures manual. Minneapolis: NCS Pearson Inc; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Rini C, et al. Psychological adaptation and birth outcomes: the role of personal resources, stress, and sociocultural context in pregnancy. Health Psychol. 1999;18(4):333–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Barnett B, Hanna B, Parker G. Life event scales for obstetric groups. J Psychosom Res. 1983;27:313–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Jomeen J, Martin C. Replicability and stability of the multidimensional model of the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs. 2007;14:319–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Boyd R, Pearson J, Blehar M. Prevention and treatment of depression in pregnancy and the postpartum period-summary of a maternal depression roundtable: a US perspective. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2002;4:79–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Morgan CD, Wiederman MW, Magnus RD. Discriminant validity of the SCL-90 dimensions of anxiety and depression. Assessment. 1998;5(2):197–201.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Bergink V, et al. Validation of the Edinburgh Depression Scale during pregnancy. J Psychosom Res. 2011;70(4):385–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Bergman K, et al. Quality of child–parent attachment moderates the impact of antenatal stress on child fearfulness. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(10):1089–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Bergman K, et al. Maternal stress during pregnancy predicts cognitive ability and fearfulness in infancy. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46:1454–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Stapleton LRT, et al. Perceived partner support in pregnancy predicts lower maternal and infant distress. J Fam Psychol. 2012;26(3):453.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Giesbrecht GF, et al. The buffering effect of social support on hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis function during pregnancy. Psychosom Med. 2013;75(9):856–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S, Sturdivant RX. Applied logistic regression, vol. 398. New York: Wiley; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  87. Kilpeläinen M, et al. Stressful life events promote the manifestation of asthma and atopic diseases. Clin Exp Allergy. 2002;32(2):256–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Wang IJ, Wen HJ, Chiang TL, Lin SJ, Guo YL. Maternal psychologic problems increased the risk of childhood atopic dermatitis. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2016;27(2):169–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Wright RJ. Stress and atopic disorders. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2005;116(6):1301–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Chida Y, Hamer M, Steptoe A. A bidirectional relationship between psychosocial factors and atopic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychosom Med. 2008;70(1):102–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Crockenberg SC, Leerkes EM. Parental acceptance, postpartum depression, and maternal sensitivity: mediating and moderating processes. J Fam Psychol. 2003;17(1):80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Goldstein LH, Diener ML, Mangelsdorf SC. Maternal characteristics and social support across the transition to motherhood: associations with maternal behavior. J Fam Psychol. 1996;10(1):60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Tietz A, Zietlow A-L, Reck C. Maternal bonding in mothers with postpartum anxiety disorder: the crucial role of subclinical depressive symptoms and maternal avoidance behaviour. Arch Womens Mental Health. 2014;17(5):433–42.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  94. Demers I, et al. Mind-mindedness in adult and adolescent mothers: relations to maternal sensitivity and infant attachment. Int J Behav Dev. 2010;34(6):529–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Pilkington PD, et al. Modifiable partner factors associated with perinatal depression and anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2015;178:165–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Kingston D, Tough S, Whitfield H. Prenatal and postpartum maternal psychological distress and infant development: a systematic review. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2012;43(5):683–714.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Raine K, et al. Antenatal interpersonal sensitivity is more strongly associated than perinatal depressive symptoms with postnatal mother–infant interaction quality. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2016;19(5):917–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Gavin NI, et al. Perinatal depression: a systematic review of prevalence and incidence. Obstet Gynecol. 2005;106(5, Part 1):1071–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Giardinelli L, et al. Depression and anxiety in perinatal period: prevalence and risk factors in an Italian sample. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2012;15(1):21–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  100. dos Santos LM, et al. Maternal mental health and social support: effect on childhood atopic and non-atopic asthma symptoms. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2012;66(11):1011–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Mangan JM, Wittich AR, Gerald LB. The potential for reducing asthma disparities through improved family and social function and modified health behaviors. CHEST J. 2007;132(5_suppl):789S–801S.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Chen E, et al. The role of the social environment in children and adolescents with asthma. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2007;176(7):644–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  103. Li F, et al. Prevalence and risk factors of childhood allergic diseases in eight metropolitan cities in China: a multicenter study. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Chang HY, Suh DI, Yang SI, Kang MJ, Lee SY, Lee E, Choi IA, Lee KS, Shin YJ, Shin YH, Kim YH. Prenatal maternal distress affects atopic dermatitis in offspring mediated by oxidative stress. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2016;138(2):468–75.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  105. Cookson H, et al. Mothers’ anxiety during pregnancy is associated with asthma in their children. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2009;123(4):847–53.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  106. Beijers R, et al. Maternal prenatal anxiety and stress predict infant illnesses and health complaints. Pediatrics. 2010;126(2):e401–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  107. Cheng TS, et al. An independent association of prenatal depression with wheezing and anxiety with rhinitis in infancy. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2015;26(8):765–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Field T. Postpartum depression effects on early interactions, parenting, and safety practices: a review. Infant Behav Dev. 2010;33(1):1–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  109. Fairbrother N, et al. Perinatal anxiety disorder prevalence and incidence. J Affect Disord. 2016;200:148–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Essex MJ, et al. Maternal stress beginning in infancy may sensitize children to later stress exposure: effects on cortisol and behavior. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;52(8):776–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Andersson NW, et al. Influence of prenatal maternal stress on umbilical cord blood cytokine levels. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2016;19(5):761–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  112. Mathilda Chiu Y-H, et al. Prenatal and postnatal maternal stress and wheeze in urban children: effect of maternal sensitization. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2012;186(2):147–54.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  113. Alkon A, et al. The ontogeny of autonomic measures in 6-and 12-month-old infants. Dev Psychobiol. 2006;48(3):197–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  114. Gunnar MR, Donzella B. Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2002;27(1):199–220.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. De Luca G, et al. Fetal and early postnatal life roots of asthma. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. 2010;23(sup3):80–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Cho H-J, et al. Effect of prenatal maternal trait anxiety on atopic dermatitis and immunoglobulin E Level: cocoa study. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2015;135(2):AB263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  117. Von Hertzen LC. Maternal stress and T-cell differentiation of the developing immune system: possible implications for the development of asthma and atopy. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2002;109(6):923–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Wright RJ, et al. Prenatal maternal stress and cord blood innate and adaptive cytokine responses in an inner-city cohort. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2010;182(1):25–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  119. Seckl JR, Holmes MC. Mechanisms of disease: glucocorticoids, their placental metabolism and fetal ‘programming’ of adult pathophysiology. Nat Clin Pract Endocrinol Metab. 2007;3(6):479–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  120. Seneviratne SL, et al. Allergen-specific CD8+ T cells and atopic disease. J Clin Investig. 2002;110(9):1283–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  121. Giesbrecht G, Campbell T, Letourneau N, Kooistra L, Kaplan B, APrON Study Team. Psychological distress and salivary cortisol covary within persons during pregnancy. Pscyhoneuroendocrinology. 2011;27(1–2):171–80.

    Google Scholar 

  122. Wright RJ, Enlow MB. Maternal stress and perinatal programming in the expression of atopy. Expert Rev Clin Immunol. 2008;4(5):535.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  123. Findlay L, Kohen D. Leave practices of parents after the birth or adoption of young children. Can Soc Trends. 2012. No. 11-008-X.

  124. Olds DL. The nurse–family partnership: an evidence-based preventive intervention. Infant Ment Health J. 2006;27(1):5–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  125. Spietz A, et al. Keys to caregiving manual. Seattle: NCAST; 1990.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ contributions

NLL, ALK, and NC drafted the manuscript. HNN conducted statistical analysis and compiled all tables and figures. LA and MJH coded and logged participant data. NLL, ALK, HNN, TSC and GFG all contributed to the formulation of study design and data analysis and interpretation. All authors read and revised the manuscript as appropriate and agreed upon this final version. All authors read and approved the final manuscript

Acknowledgements

Generous guidance in accurate formation of variables was provided by Dr. Allan Becker. We would also like to thank the participants of the Alberta Pregnancy Outcomes and Nutrition (APrON) Fetal Programming sub-study for their effort and time commitment given to supporting this research.

Competing interests

Authors employed by the University of Calgary and University of Alberta.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets analyzed for this study are not publicly available due to confidentiality agreements but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethics approval was obtained from the Conjoint Health Research Ethics Board at the University of Calgary and the Health Research Ethics Board at the University of Alberta, both in Canada. Participation in this study was strictly voluntary and only undertaken once informed consent was provided by each participating individual.

Funding

Funding for this project was obtained from NCE AllerGen, a CIHR Operating Grant, and the University of Calgary Markin Summer Studentship.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Consortia

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicole L. Letourneau.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Letourneau, N.L., Kozyrskyj, A.L., Cosic, N. et al. Maternal sensitivity and social support protect against childhood atopic dermatitis. Allergy Asthma Clin Immunol 13, 26 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13223-017-0199-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13223-017-0199-4

Keywords